Dictionary Definition
proprioceptive adj : of or relating to
proprioception
User Contributed Dictionary
English
Adjective
- Of or pertaining to proprioception
Extensive Definition
Proprioception ( PRO-pree-o-SEP-shun); from
Latin
proprius, meaning "one's own" and perception) is the sense of the relative position of
neighbouring parts of the body. Unlike the six exteroceptive senses
(sight, taste, smell, touch,
hearing,
and balance)
by which we perceive the outside world, and interoceptive senses, by
which we perceive the pain and the stretching of internal organs,
proprioception is a third distinct sensory modality that provides
feedback solely on the status of the body internally. It is the
sense that indicates whether the body is moving with required
effort, as well as where the various parts of the body are located
in relation to each other.
Conscious and unconscious proprioception
In humans, a distinction is made between conscious proprioception and unconscious proprioception:- Conscious proprioception is communicated by the posterior column-medial lemniscus pathway to the cerebrum.
- Unconscious proprioception is communicated primarily via the dorsal spinocerebellar tract, to the cerebellum.
Tests for proprioception
In domestic animals, there are a number of relatively specific tests of the subjects ability to propriorecept, these are used in the diagnosis of neurological disorders. These tests include the visual and tactile placing reflexes.History of study
The position-movement sensation was originally described in 1557 by Julius Caesar Scaliger as a 'sense of locomotion'. Much later in 1826 Charles Bell expounded the idea of a 'muscle sense' and this is credited with being one of the first physiologic feedback mechanisms. Bell's idea was that commands were being carried from the brain to the muscles, and that reports on the muscle's condition would be sent in the reverse direction. Later, in 1880, Henry Charlton Bastian suggested 'kinaesthesia' instead of 'muscle sense' on the basis that some of the afferent information (back to the brain) was coming from other structures including tendons, joints, skin, and muscle. In 1889, Alfred Goldscheider suggested a classification of kinaesthesia into 3 types: muscle, tendon, and articular sensitivity.In 1906, Charles
Scott Sherrington published a landmark work which introduced
the terms 'proprioception', 'interoception', and 'exteroception'.
The 'exteroceptors' were the organs responsible for information
from outside the body such as the eyes, ears, mouth, and skin. The
interoceptors then gave information about the internal organs,
while 'proprioception' was awareness of movement derived from
muscular, tendon, and articular sources. Such a system of
classification has kept physiologists and anatomists searching for
specialised nerve endings which transmit data on joint capsule and
muscle tension (such as muscle spindles and Pacini
corpuscles).
Proprioception vs. kinesthesia
Kinesthesia is another term that is often used interchangeably with proprioception, though use of the term "kinesthesia" can place a greater emphasis on motion.Some differentiate the kinesthetic sense from
proprioception by excluding the sense of equilibrium or balance
from kinesthesia. An inner ear infection, for example, might
degrade the sense of balance. This would degrade the proprioceptive
sense, but not the kinesthetic sense. The infected person would be
able to walk, but only by using the sense of sight to maintain
balance; the person would be unable to walk with eyes closed.
Proprioception and kinaesthesia are seen as
interrelated and there is considerable disagreement regarding the
definition of these terms. Some of this difficulty stems from
Sherrington's original description of joint position sense (or the
ability to determine where a particular body part exactly is in
space) and kinaesthesia (or the sensation that the body part has
moved) under a more general heading of proprioception. Clinical
aspects of proprioception are measured in tests that measure a
subject's ability to detect an externally imposed passive movement,
or the ability to reposition a joint to a predetermined position.
Often it is assumed that the ability of one of these aspects will
be related to another; unfortunately, experimental evidence
suggests there is no strong relation between these two aspects.
This suggests that, while these components may well be related in a
cognitive manner, they seem to be separate physiologically.
Much of the foregoing work is dependent on the
notion that proprioception is essentially a feedback mechanism;
that is, the body moves (or is moved) and then the information
about this is returned to the brain, whereby subsequent adjustments
could be made. More recent work into the mechanism of ankle sprains
suggests that the role of reflexes may be more limited due to their
long latencies (even at the spinal cord level) as ankle sprain
events occur in perhaps 100msec or less. Accordingly, a model has
been proposed to include a 'feedforward' component of
proprioception where the subject will also have central information
about the body's position prior to attaining it.
Kinesthesia is a key component in muscle
memory and hand-eye
coordination, and training can improve this sense (see blind
contour drawing). The ability to swing a golf club or to catch
a ball requires a finely-tuned sense of the position of the joints.
This sense needs to become automatic through training to enable a
person to concentrate on other aspects of performance, such as
maintaining motivation or seeing where other people are.
Basis of proprioceptive sense
The inititation of proprioception is the activation of a proprioreceptor in the periphery The proprioceptive sense is believed to be composed of information from sensory neurons located in the inner ear (motion and orientation) and in the stretch receptors located in the muscles and the joint-supporting ligaments (stance). There are specific nerve receptors for this form of perception termed "proprioreceptors", just as there are specific receptors for pressure, light, temperature, sound, and other sensory experiences. Proprioreceptors are sometimes known as adequate stimuli receptors.Although it was known that finger kinesthesia
relies on skin sensation, recent research has found that
kinesthesia-based haptic
perception strongly relies on the forces experienced during touch.
This research allows the creation of "virtual", illusory haptic
shapes with different perceived qualities.
Applications
misleading sectionLaw enforcement
Proprioception is tested by American police officers using the field sobriety test where the subject is required to touch his or her nose with eyes closed. People with normal proprioception may make an error of no more than 20 millimetres. People suffering from impaired proprioception (a symptom of moderate to severe alcohol intoxication) fail this test due to difficulty locating their limbs in space relative to their noses.Learning
Proprioception is what allows someone to learn to walk in complete darkness without losing balance. During the learning of any new skill, sport, or art, it is usually necessary to become familiar with some proprioceptive tasks specific to that activity. Without the appropriate integration of proprioceptive input, an artist would not be able to brush paint onto a canvas without looking at the hand as it moved the brush over the canvas; it would be impossible to drive an automobile because a motorist would not be able to steer or use the foot pedals while looking at the road ahead; a person could not touch type or perform ballet; and people would not even be able to walk without watching where they put their feet.Oliver Sacks
once reported the case of a young woman who lost her proprioception
due to a viral infection of her spinal cord.
At first she was not able to move properly at all or even control
her tone of voice (as voice modulation is primarily
proprioceptive). Later she relearned by using her sight (watching
her feet) and vestibulum (or inner ear) only
for movement while using hearing to judge voice modulation. She
eventually acquired a stiff and slow movement and nearly normal
speech, which is believed to be the best possible in the absence of
this sense. She could not judge effort involved in picking up
objects and would grip them painfully to be sure she didn't drop
them.
Training
The proprioceptive sense can be sharpened through study of many disciplines. The Alexander Technique uses the study of movement to enhance kinesthetic judgment of effort and location. Juggling trains reaction time, spatial location, and efficient movement. Standing on a wobble board or balance board is often used to retrain or increase proprioception abilities, particularly as physical therapy for ankle or knee injuries. Standing on one leg (stork standing) and various other body-position challenges are also used in such disciplines as Yoga or Wing Chun. In addition, the slow, focused movements of Tai Chi practice provide an environment whereby the proprioceptive information being fed back to the brain stimulates an intense, dynamic "listening environment" to further enhance mind / body integration. Several studies have shown that the efficacy of these types of training is challenged by closing the eyes, because the eyes give invaluable feedback to establishing the moment-to-moment information of balance. There are even specific devices designed for proprioception training, such as the Proprioceptor system, which consists of shoes with specially designed balls on the soles to make athletes work harder to balance.Impairment
Apparently, temporary loss or impairment of
proprioception may happen periodically during growth, mostly during
adolescence. Growth that might also influence this would be large
increases or drops in bodyweight/size due to fluctuations of fat
(liposuction,
rapid fat
loss, rapid fat
gain) and muscle content (bodybuilding, anabolic
steroids, catabolisis/starvation). It can also
occur to those who gain new levels of flexibility, stretching, and contortion. A limb's being in
a new range of motion never experienced (or at least, not for a
long time since youth perhaps) can disrupt one's sense of location
of that limb. Possible experiences include these: suddenly feeling
that feet or legs are missing from one's mental self-image; needing
to look down at one's limbs to be sure they are still there; and
falling down while walking, especially when attention is focused
upon something other than the act of walking.
Proprioception is occasionally impaired
spontaneously, especially when one is tired. One's body may appear
too large or too small, or parts of the body may appear distorted
in size. Similar effects can sometimes occur during epilepsy or migraine auras. These effects are presumed
to arise from abnormal stimulation of the part of the parietal
cortex of the brain
involved with integrating information from different parts of the
body.
Proprioceptive illusions can also be induced,
such as the Pinocchio
illusion.
The proprioceptive sense is often unnoticed
because humans will adapt to a continuously-present stimulus; this
is called habituation, desensitization, or
adaptation. The
effect is that proprioceptive sensory impressions disappear, just
as a scent can disappear over time. One practical advantage of this
is that unnoticed actions or sensation continue in the background
while an individual's attention can move to another concern. The
Alexander Technique addresses these issues.
People who have a limb amputated may still have a
confused sense of that limb existence on their body, known as
phantom
limb syndrome. Phantom sensations can occur as passive
proprioceptive sensations of the limb's presence, or more active
sensations such as perceived movement, pressure, pain, itching, or
temperature. The etiology of the phantom limb phenomenon was
disputed in 2006, but some consensus existed in favour of neurological
(e.g. neural signal bleed across a preexisting sensory map, as
posited by V.S.
Ramachandran) over psychological
explanations. Phantom sensations and phantom pain may also occur
after the removal of body parts other than the limbs, such as after
amputation of the breast, extraction of a tooth (phantom tooth
pain), or removal of an eye (phantom
eye syndrome).
Temporary impairment of proprioception has also
been known to occur from an overdose of vitamin B6
(pyridoxine and pyridoxamine). Most of the impaired function
returns to normal shortly after the intake of vitamins returns to
normal. Impairment can also be caused by cytotoxic factors such as
chemotherapy.
It has been proposed that even common tinnitus and the attendant
hearing frequency-gaps masked by the perceived sounds may cause
erroneous proprioceptive information to the balance and
comprehension centers of the brain, precipitating mild
confusion.
Proprioception is permanently impaired in
patients who suffer from joint hypermobility or Ehlers-Danlos
Syndrome (a genetic condition that results in weak connective
tissue throughout the body). It can also be permanently impaired
from viral infections as reported by Sacks. The catastrophic effect
of major proprioceptive loss is reviewed by Robles-De-La-Torre
(2006).
External links
- Somatosensory Pathways from the Body from the Washington University School of Medicine's Neuroscience Tutorial
- The Proprioception Trust
- Joint & Bone - Ehlers-Danlos/Joint Hypermobility Syndrome - Proprioception
- Humans have six senses, why does everyone think we only have five? at Everything2
- Proprioception this essay by Charles Wolfe takes its cue from such thinkers & artists as Charles Olson, Merleau-Ponty, J.J. Gibson, and Andy Clark to illustrate the view of the "priority of dynamic embodied activity over isolated 'mental' and 'physical' regions" to define this concept
- WNYC - Radio Lab: Where Am I? (May 05, 2006) radio program looks at the relationship between the brain and the body
- The Dancers Mind ABC (Aust) podcast on the nature of proprioception.
proprioceptive in German:
Tiefensensibilität
proprioceptive in Spanish: Propiocepción
proprioceptive in French: Proprioception
proprioceptive in Italian: Propriocezione
proprioceptive in Dutch: Proprioceptie
proprioceptive in Polish: Propriocepcja
proprioceptive in Portuguese:
Propriocepção
proprioceptive in Russian: Кинестезия
proprioceptive in Finnish: Asentoaisti
proprioceptive in Swedish: Proprioception
proprioceptive in Chinese:
肌肉運動知覺